Thirty three University of Limerick students were asked to participate in a research concerning implicit and explicit attitudes toward smoking-related social identities, for example smokers and non-smokers. Implicit Relational Assessement Procedure (IRAP) was enlisted in this research to understand an individual’s implicit attitudes. Past research has indicated that we are often unaware of these attitudes as self presentation strategies mediate implicit attitudes if they are socially undesirable. A 2 x 2 mixed design was entailed, the DV’s were response latencies on the IRAP and attitude responses on the Explicit’s scale. The research asked participants to indicate their smoking history (which was an IV) and they had to respond to a computerised task both in a consistent and inconsistent manner, depending on their behavioural history (the second IV). There was two practise blocks and six test blocks, participants had to score over eighty per cent and respond to the stimuli within two seconds. IRAP eliminates social buffering and explicit attitudes were gathered in an attitude thermometer scale.
The main findings indicate that smokers respond to smokers and non-smokers positively both implicitly and explicitly. Non-smokers respond implicitly and explicitly in a more positive manner toward non-smokers. However non-smokers are implicitly ambiguous toward smokers, yet explicitly they condone smokers. There is an interaction effect in the Explicit scale as non-smokers score higher in being anti-smoker than pro non-smoker.
Within society, smoking has always been a topical issue which has experienced significant attitude change in the last fifty years. In the past, smoking was considered a positive social behaviour; however research linking smoking to throat and lung cancer has led to negative attitudes toward smoking and smokers. As smoking is considered detrimental to one’s health, the Irish government have implemented a number of anti-smoker strategies, for example advertisement campaigns on the negative effects of smoking, high taxes on cigarettes.
However the most influential of these was the smoking ban which was sanctioned in 2003. This law prohibits smoking in public locations, where smokers are forced outside consequently. It is now of the social opinion that it is “rude to light up without asking the permission of those sharing your breathing space” (Euranet, 2010).
This research elaborates from the study of Vahey et al who investigated smoking-related social identity preferences within adolescents; their findings indicated that adolescent smokers are more likely to hold positive regard for smokers, in comparison to non-smokers. However non-smokers impression of smokers and non-smokers did not differ, this may be due to peer pressure i.e. striving to fit in.
To expand from this study, our research will be carried out in the adult population as peer pressure is less evident at this stage. Within this research it is hoped to reveal both implicit and explicit attitudes toward smoking-related social identity. Implicit attitudes relate to “hidden nature of social attitudes” which are not prevalent explicitly due to “self presentation strategies,” (). In order to uncover these implicit attitudes, the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) was used, the hypothesis of the experiment states: Participants should have faster response latencies in consistent rather than inconsistent measures. Shorter response times indicate that the stimuli are consistent with one’s attitude. IRAP presents inconsistent stimuli, it is expected that participants will take longer to respond as this does not correspond with their views which eliminates social buffering.
The research hypothesis affirms that non-smokers will both implicitly and explicitly regard themselves positively as being a non-smoker is considered socially desirable. Non-smoker’s will be more negative toward smokers implicitly and explicitly due to social attitudes, developed through the introduction of the smoking ban, adverts etc. Smokers will regard themselves positively as smoking is considered part of their social identity but also deem non-smokers positively but not as highly as non-smokers. There will be no interaction effect between smokers and non-smokers implicitly and explicitly.
The sample consisted of 33 undergraduate students from University of Limerick; including eight males and twenty five females. There were 18 smokers and 15non-smokers, all of whom participated voluntarily.
A questionnaire containing demographics, identified their smoking status was employed, this also served as a primer as it asked about different aspects of smoking for example “have you ever lived with anyone that smoked?”
IRAP was used to understand the implicit attitudes associated with smokers and non-smokers. This is a computerised task which accesses automatic responses and understands natural verbal relations (Barnes-Holmes, 2010). IRAP requires the subject to combine the target stimuli (smoker, non smoker) and the positive or negative responses to attain the desired consistent or inconsistent response.
Within this research, the data were coded by classifying “non-smokers are cool” or “smokers are lame” in the consistent block. The inconsistent block would be “non-smokers are lame” or “smokers are independent.” Another questionnaire was administered post- IRAP to analyse explicit attitudes toward smokers and non-smokers.
Cool
Lame
Independent
Boring
Respected
Needy
Attractive
Loner
Fun
Weak
Popular
Loser
A 2 x 2 mixed method design was utilised within this research. The DV’s were responses times on the IRAP and responses in the explicit thermometer scale (Explicits scale). The IV’s in the study were smoking history (smoker/non smoker) and one’s smoking behavioural history (consistent or inconsistent).
The researcher first administered the first questionnaire which primed attitudes of smokers and non-smokers. The participants were asked to read the IRAP instructions where they had to answer either Pro-smoker/anti non-smoker or pro non-smoker/anti-smoker. The study required participants to complete two practise blocks and six test blocks. If one is unable to complete these practise blocks, IRAP gives the participant the opportunity to have four more tries, after this they were thanked for their participation and their data is disregarded. IRAP outlines that participants must score over eighty per cent and respond to each trial in less than two seconds. Participants respond by indicating true (pressing the key D) or false (pressing the key K) which interchange during the task. The researcher asked the participant to slow down in the practise blocks if they are making a lot of errors, so to understand the main principal of the research.
It was necessary to have strict lab conditions as the task required complete attention, where possible all distractions were eliminated for example noise, mobile phone etc. IRAP compares the response speed from the consistent and inconsistent blocks. If a positive difference is attained one is more likely to express a pro non-smoker attitude, while if the result is negative one is more associated with expressing a pro-smoker attitude.
The Explicit questionnaire gathered information on the participant’s explicit attitude toward smokers and non smokers on a 0-100 attitude thermometer scale.
A Two way 2 (smoking history: Smoker or Non-smoker) X 2 (Behavioural: Consistent vs. Inconsistent) mixed ANOVA was conducted on the IRAP scores.
There is a significant linear trend on the IRAP scale F(1, 31)= 38.503, p< 0.001, η2=0.554, over the mean values for each level of the factor. The interaction between the smokers and non-smokers and the explicit scale is not significant, F(1, 31)= 0.447, p= .509, η2= .014 over the mean values for each level of the factor.
The main effect whether the smoker versus the non-smoker scores differed on the IRAP was significant F(1, 31) =7.408, p= .011, η2 = 0.193. The interaction between smokers and non smokers social identity preferences was not significant F(1, 31) = 3.413, p=.074, η2 =0.099.
Smokers scored lower (mean= -0.37) than non-smoker’s (mean= -0.14) in the Smoker_D IRAP. The mean difference between conditions was -0.23 and the confidence interval for the estimated population mean difference is between -4.01 and -0.57. An independent t-test showed a difference between conditions was significant (t=2.70, df=31, p=.011 two tailed.)
Smokers scored lower (mean= -0.37) than non-smoker’s (mean= -0.14) in the Non-smoker_D IRAP. The mean difference between conditions was -0.23 and the confidence interval for the estimated population mean difference is between -4.01 and -0.57. An independent t-test showed a difference between conditions was significant (t=2.70, df=31, p=.011 two tailed.)
The average time it took for smokers to respond to Smoker_D was shorter than of the non-smoker_D trials. The mean difference between conditions was -0.415ms and the 95% confidence interval for the estimated population mean difference is between 0.586 and 0.254. A paired t-test showed that the difference between conditions was significant (t= -5.162, df= 16, p<.001, two tailed.
The average time it took for non-smokers to respond to Non-smoker_D was shorter than of the smoker_D trials. The mean difference between conditions was -.33385ms and the 95% confidence interval for the estimated population mean difference is between -0.539 and -0.128. A paired t-test showed that the difference between conditions was significant (t= 4.36, df= 16, p<.001, two tailed.
A Two way 2 (smoking history: Smoker or Non-smoker) X 2 (Behavioural: Consistent vs. Inconsistent) mixed ANOVA was conducted on the Explicit preferences toward smoking related social identity ratings.
There is a significant linear trend on the explicit scale, F(1, 30)= 7.952, p= .008 η2 = .210, over the mean values for each level of the factor. There is also a significant interaction between the smokers and non-smokers and the explicit scale, F(1, 30)= 7.522, p= .007, η2= .220, over the mean values for each level of the factor.
The main effect whether smoker versus the non-smoker scores differed on the Explicit scale was significant F(1, 30) = 13.952, p=.001, η2 = 0.317. There was a significant interaction between non-smokers and smokers on the explicit’s scale F(1, 30) = 12.506, p=0.001, η2 = .294.
Smokers scored positively (mean= -0.696) than non-smoker’s who responded negatively (mean= 0.711) in the Explicit Smoker Scale. The mean difference between conditions was 1.407 and the confidence interval for the estimated population mean difference is between -2.20 and -0.605. An independent t-test showed a difference between conditions was significant (t=-3.586, df=30, p=.001 two tailed.)
Smokers scored less positively (mean= .657) in comparison to the non-smoker’s(mean= 0.690) in the Explicit Non-Smoker Scale. The mean difference between conditions was -0.33 and the confidence interval for the estimated population mean difference is between -3.99 and -0.333. An independent t-test showed a difference between conditions was not significant (t=–0.185, df=30, p=.855 two tailed.)
Smokers responded more positively to the Explicit Smoker Scale (-0.696) compared to the Explicit Non-Smoker Scale (0.6578). The mean difference between conditions was 1.353 and the 95% confidence interval for the estimated population mean difference is between 0.695 and 2.01. A paired t-test showed that the difference between conditions was significant (t= 4.36, df= 16, p<.001, two tailed).
Non-smokers responded positively toward the Explicit Non-smoker scale (0.6909), however they scored extremely negative toward the Explicit Smoker scale (0.711). The mean difference between the conditions was -0.02 and the 95% confidence interval for the estimated population mean difference was between -0.729 and 0.752. A paired t-test showed that the difference between conditions was not significant (t= -0.056, df= 14, p=0.956, two tailed),
Pearson’s Correlation of Non Smoker D (IRAP) and Explicit Non-Smoker
Non-Smoker_D
Explicit Non Smoker
Pearson Correlation
-.008
Sig (2-tailed)
.965
Pearson’s Correlation of Smoker_D (IRAP) and Explicit Smoker
Explicit Smoker
Pearson’s Correlation
Sig (2-tailed)
Overall there was a significant difference between smokers and non smokers on the IRAP in the consistent and inconsistent variable; however the effect size was relatively small. Smokers were found to be positive toward smokers and non-smokers both on the IRAP and explicit’s scale. Non-smokers were consistent holding positive regard of themselves. There was a difference in the non-smokers implicit and explicit responses towards smokers, their explicit responses indicate negative regard for smokers, yet implicitly they were ambiguous. There was an interaction effect in the explicit scale as non-smokers were more negative toward smokers than positive toward non-smokers. There was no significant relationship between responses on the explicit smoker scale and implicit smoker_D scale. Likewise no significant correlation was evident between the explicit non-smoker scale and the non-smoker_D scale.
FIGURE 1; Responses on IRAP demonstrating preferences toward smoking-social identity.
FIGURE 2: Smokers and Non-Smokers responses on the Explicit Thermometer scale,
Within this research, we fail to accept our null hypothesis, as smokers regard themselves and non-smokers positively both explicitly and implicitly, which is consistent with the findings of Vahey et al. Within our second hypothesis, we accept parts; it was found that non-smokers will score higher in the pro-non smoker condition, as it is relevant to their behavioural history and social identity. However the research demonstrates that non-smokers will explicitly express negative attitudes toward smokers, yet implicitly respond in an ambiguous manner. The IRAP methodology is proficient as it reveals social buffering prevalent within non-smokers. The interaction effect indicated that non-smoker were explicitly marginally more negative toward smokers than positive toward non-smoker’s. This may be due to the more prevalent negative social attitude toward smokers, while non-smokers receive less positive reinforcement.
In previous research non-smoker adolescents did not implicitly or explicitly condone smokers due to peer pressure as this behaviour is considered cool (Vahey et al, 2010). Our results differ as the adult population are more influenced by the negative stereotype of smokers which is manifested by the governmental laws, advertising etc. People are more likely to express this negative opinion as it is socially desirable however they may not inherently agree.
One of the strengths of this research is that implicit attitudes are exposed concerning smoking-related identities, which increases the relevance of IRAP. For instance this research identifies social buffering as non-smokers conflict in their implicit and explicit attitudes toward smokers.
There are a number of shortcomings within this research for example there is a small effect size, which reduces the generalisability of this research to the general population. This study was not carried out in a lab setting which could have affected people’s responses. For instance, there may have been more distractions prevalent which could have delayed people’s responses on the IRAP in the consistent and inconsistent condition. An alternative shortcoming to be considered is that some of the sample may have been social smokers and may not have taken the smoker-identity.
Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Stewart, I., & Boles S., (2010) A Sketch of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) and the Relational Elaboration and Coherence (REC) Model. The Psychological Record, (60) p. 527-542
Vahey, N., Boles, S., & Barnes-Holmes, D., (2010) “Measuring Adolescents’ Smoking-related Social Identity Preferences with the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) for the First Time: A Starting Point that Explains Later IRAP Evolutions” National University of Ireland Maynooth and Oregan Research Institute p. 1-45
Within Subjects ANOVA
df
F
Sig.
IRAP
Sphericity Assumed
1
38.503
.000
Greenhouse-Geisser
1.000
38.503
.000
Huynh-Feldt
1.000
38.503
.000
Lower-bound
1.000
38.503
.000
IRAP interaction with Smoker Non-smoker
Sphericity Assumed
1
.447
.509
Greenhouse-Geisser
1.000
.447
.509
Huynh-Feldt
1.000
.447
.509
Lower-bound
1.000
.447
.509
Error(IRAP)
Sphericity Assumed
31
Greenhouse-Geisser
31.000
Huynh-Feldt
31.000
Lower-bound
31.000
Df
F
Sig.
Intercept
1
3.413
.074
Smoker/Non-smoker
1
7.408
.011
Error
31
Within Subjects ANOVA
Df
F
Sig.
Explicit Scale
Sphericity Assumed
1
7.952
.008
Greenhouse-Geisser
1.000
7.952
.008
Huynh-Feldt
1.000
7.952
.008
Lower-bound
1.000
7.952
.008
Explicit Scale interaction with Smoker Non-smoker
Sphericity Assumed
1
.891
.007
Greenhouse-Geisser
1.000
.891
.007
Huynh-Feldt
1.000
.891
.007
Lower-bound
1.000
.891
.007
Error(Explicit)
Sphericity Assumed
30
Greenhouse-Geisser
30.000
Huynh-Feldt
30.000
Df
F
Sig.
Intercept
1
12.506
.001
Smoker/Non-smoker
1
13.952
.001
Error
30
95% Confidence Interval of the Difference
T
Df
Sig (2-tailed)
Mean Difference
Std Error Diff
Lower
Non Smoker_D
Equal variances assumed
-1.513
30
.141
-15451
.10214
-3.6310
Smoker_D
Equal variances assumed
-2.684
30
.012
-.23642
.08809
-.41631
95% Confidence Interval of the Difference
t
Df
Sig (2-tailed)
Mean Difference
Std Error Diff
Lower
Explicit Smoker
Equal variances assumed
-3.586
30
.001
-1.40719
.39243
-2.20865
Explicit Non-Smoker
Equal variances assumed
-.185
30
.855
-.03316
.17937
-.39947
Mean
N
Std. Deviation
Std. Error Mean
Pair 1
Smoker_D
-.3726
17
.22809
.05532
Non_Smoker_D
.0432
17
.26499
.06427
Paired Differences
t
df
Sig. (2-tailed)
Mean
Std. Deviation
Std. Error Mean
95% Confidence Interval of the Difference
Lower
Upper
Smoker_D – Non_Smoker_D
-.41575
.33209
.08054
-.58650
-.24500
-5.162
16
Mean
N
Std. Deviation
Pair 1
Smoker_D
-1.362
15
.27026
Non_Smoker_D
.1977
15
.31286
Paired Differences
t
df
Mean
Std. Deviation
Std. Error Mean
95% Confidence Interval of the Difference
Lower
Upper
Smoker_D – Non_Smoker_D
-.33385
.37074
.09572
-.53916
-.12854
-3.488
14
.004
You have to be 100% sure of the quality of your product to give a money-back guarantee. This describes us perfectly. Make sure that this guarantee is totally transparent.
Read moreEach paper is composed from scratch, according to your instructions. It is then checked by our plagiarism-detection software. There is no gap where plagiarism could squeeze in.
Read moreThanks to our free revisions, there is no way for you to be unsatisfied. We will work on your paper until you are completely happy with the result.
Read moreYour email is safe, as we store it according to international data protection rules. Your bank details are secure, as we use only reliable payment systems.
Read moreBy sending us your money, you buy the service we provide. Check out our terms and conditions if you prefer business talks to be laid out in official language.
Read more